Rate Thread
  • 0 Vote(s) - 0 Average
  • 1
  • 2
  • 3
  • 4
  • 5
statistal sample of 40
#1
A paper from the University of Austin TX, the first pointing out negative characteristics of children raised by gay parents was published by Mark Regnerus
[Image: regnerus_markX400.jpg]

reference his findings, especially Table 2

worth knowing:
-his work based on a sample size of 40 people, a few decimal places from anything significant.
-serious cash from anti gay groups (nearly a million USD).
Reply

#2
This cannot be taken seriously, 40 people???? Nothing could possibly come from a study so tiny, not even a hint.

What I do approve of is anti-gay groups spending their money on things like this, because it is a titanical waste of money, the results of which no human being on earth will actual find out about these studies UNLESS they go looking for it.

Spend away, go broke I say Smile
Reply

#3
I learned many years ago when I debated on a semi professional level to always figure out the source for any of the "facts" I used...meaning...who exactly financed the paper?

You can get ANY conclusion you want if you have the $$$$$.

The problem is...not alot of people will figure this out...and in the end enough people will just accept what they hear and read as a fact just BECAUSE they read it or heard it or saw it on Fox News.

I remember on Fox when a scandal would happen with a Republican they would actually put a (D) after the name on the screen to let their viewers think it was a Democrat. they did it on many occasions.
Reply

#4
I agree. 40 people is too small of a "Statistical" anything...

It's more opinionated, then it is statistical. There must be a reason why he only got 40 people, probably cause the rest thought it was complete BS .

My cousin is Gay[and is the only other gay guy in my family, aside from me] and he raised his younger sisters[my cousins] just fine!

People as a whole think that Feminine men will raise Feminine boys or Masculine women will raise Masculine girls... that's just idiotic...

I saw two women in a magazine the other day, and they adopted a baby and at the time one of my mom's male friends was over[I've known him from young] and he said; "That poor child" and I was like why?

He said, every child needs a Mom and a Dad, otherwise it won't work...

I almost went off it, because I personally would love to have a child one day and for him to say that my child is not going to be raised properly, because I'm not with a woman? Screw that.

As long as the parents love their child with all of themselves, and teach them the right way, then gender should have nothing to do with it.

I was raised by a single mother, and I came out just fine, without a Dad. I despise Alcohol & Drugs, I'm not in a gang, I love animals and music, I am trustworthy and kind and I am not violent. Alot of so called "normal" families, with a mom and dad, produce children who become murderers or rapists, so they can't say that as long as you have a Mom and a Dad, that the child will turn out perfect.

So, I think there's more than one way to skin a cat.
Reply

#5
Yes there is a small sample size but my concerns are elsewhere, what type of statistical tests did the paper do? They only did means and ranges from my view! Usually in a investigation like this you need to do a proper statistical test such as a parametric or non parametric test to compare something called a p value. I'm gonna have a read of this further because statistically it doesn't seem relevant lol
Reply

#6
One CAN do a statistical analysis with a sample of 40.

The trouble with same samples is chance is such a big factor one needs a really strong finding not to be reasonable explained by pure chance. Small samples bias a study in favour of finding no difference between opposite-sex couples raising kids and same-sex couples.

The real trouble with the study is not the small sample size it's is the lengths Regnerus went to enlarge his sample. He included everyone who had a parent who had ever had any same-sex relationship during their childhood. This included children who had grown up in a variety of unstable, dysfunctional households. Most had lived with the same-sex partner for less than 4 months. He compared that group with heterosexual couples who had stayed together throughout their children's lives. His group of children of gay fathers and lesbian mothers is so heterogenous that the fact they did worse is meaningless.

All the study shows is that (as has been repeated demonstrated) family instability hurts children.


Sent from my iPhone using Tapatalk
Fred

Life is what happens while you are busy making other plans.
Reply

#7
QueenOdi Wrote:I agree. 40 people is too small of a "Statistical" anything...

It's more opinionated, then it is statistical. There must be a reason why he only got 40 people, probably cause the rest thought it was complete BS .

You try to find at random 40 adults with gay parents.

QueenOdi Wrote:I was raised by a single mother, and I came out just fine, without a Dad.

If 40 is too small, what is the use of 1?


Sent from my iPhone using Tapatalk
Fred

Life is what happens while you are busy making other plans.
Reply

#8
Mark Regnerus got paid very well for his study for a professional clinical situation not mother and child.
Reply

#9
I wasn't sure which thread to add this to but I thought this article should be added to the discussion.

200+ PhDs And MDs Question Scholarly Merit Of Regnerus Study

Quote:Letter to the editors and advisory editors of Social Science Research

As researchers and scholars, many of whom with extensive experience in quantitative and qualitative research in family structures and child outcomes, we write to raise serious concerns about the most recent issue of Social Science Research and the set of papers focused on parenting by lesbians and gay men. In this regard, we have particular concern about Mark Regnerus’ paper entitled “How different are the adult children of parents who have same-sex relationships? Findings from the New Family Structures Study.”

LGBT parenting is a highly politicized topic. While the presence of a vibrant and controversial public debate should in no way censor scholarship, it should compel the academy to hold scholarship around that topic to our most rigorous standards. We are very concerned that these standards were not upheld in this issue or with this paper, given the apparently expedited process of publication and the decision to publish commentaries on the paper by scholars who were directly involved with the study and have limited experience in LGBT parenting research. We also have serious concerns about the scholarly merit of this paper.

In this letter, we detail the specific concerns that lead us to request that you publicly disclose the reasons for both the expedited peer review process of this clearly controversial paper and the choice of commentators invited to submit critiques. We further request that you invite scholars with specific expertise in LGBT parenting issues to submit a detailed critique of the paper and accompanying commentaries for publication in the next issue of the journal.

We question the process by which this paper was submitted, reviewed, and accepted for publication. The paper was received by the journal on February 1, 2012. A revision was received on February 29, and the paper was accepted on March 12. This suggests that the peer review process and substantive revisions occurred within a period of just five weeks. According to the peer review policy of the Social Science Research website hosted by Elsevier, the first step of the review process is an initial manuscript evaluation by the editor. Once deemed to meet minimum criteria, at least 2 experts are secured for a peer review. The website states that, “Typically manuscripts are reviewed within 2-3 months of submission but substantially longer review times are not uncommon” and that “Revised manuscripts are usually returned to the initial referees upon receipt.” Clearly, Dr. Regnerus’ paper was returned to him very quickly, because he had time to revise the manuscript and get it back to the journal by February 29th. Further, it appears that a second substantive peer review may not have occurred as the paper was accepted just two weeks after the revision was submitted.

The five-week submission to acceptance length was much shorter than all of the other articles published in the July 2012 issue. The average period of review for papers published in this issue was more than a year and the median review time was more than ten months. As we note below, there are substantial concerns about the merits of this paper, and these concerns should have been identified through a thorough and rigorous peer review process.

We further question the selection of commenters for the Regnerus paper. While Cynthia Osborne and Paul Amato are certainly well-respected scholars, they are also both active participants in the Regnerus study. According to her curriculum vitae, Dr. Osborne is a Co-Principal Investigator of the New Family Structure Survey. Dr. Amato served as a paid consultant on the advisory group convened to provide insights into study design and methods. Perhaps more importantly, neither Osborne nor Amato have ever published work that considers LGBT family or parenting issues. A cursory examination of this body of literature would reveal a wide range of scholars who are much more qualified to evaluate the merits of this study and were neither directly involved in the study design nor compensated for that involvement.

We have substantial concerns about the merits of this paper and question whether it actually uses methods and instruments that answer the research questions posed in the paper. The author claims that the purpose of the analysis is to begin to address the question, “Do the children of gay and lesbian parents look comparable to those of their heterosexual counterparts?” (p. 755). He creates several categories of “family type”, including “lesbian mother” and “gay father” as well as “divorced late,” “stepfamily,” and “single-parent.” But, as the author notes, for those respondents who indicated that a parent had a “same-sex relationship,” these categories were collapsed to boost sample size:

That is, a small minority of respondents might fit more than one group. I have, however, forced their mutual exclusivity here for analytic purposes. For example, a respondent whose mother had a same-sex relationship might also qualify in Group 5 or Group 7, but in this case my analytical interest is in maximizing the sample size of Groups 2 and 3 so the respondent would be placed in Group 2 (LMs). Since Group 3 (GFs) is the smallest and most difficult to locate randomly in the population, its composition trumped that of others, even LMs. (There were 12 cases of respondents who reported both a mother and a father having a same-sex relationship; all are analyzed here as GFs, after ancillary analyses revealed comparable exposure to both their mother and father).

By doing this, the author is unable to distinguish between the impact of having a parent who has had a continuous same-sex relationship from the impact of having same-sex parents who broke-up from the impact of living in a same-sex stepfamily from the impact of living with a single parent who may have dated a same-sex partner; each of these groups are included in a single “lesbian mother” or “gay father” group depending on the gender of the parent who had a same-sex relationship. Specifically, this paper fails to distinguish family structure and family instability. Thus, it fails to distinguish, for children whose parents ever had a same-sex relationship experience, the associations due to family structure from the associations due to family stability. However, he does attempt to distinguish family structure from family instability for the children of different-sex parents by identifying children who lived in an intact biological family. To make a group equivalent to the group he labels as having “lesbian” or “gay” parents, the author should have grouped all other respondents together and included those who lived in an intact biological family with those who ever experienced divorce, or whose parents ever had a different-sex romantic relationship. That seems absurd to family structure researchers, yet that type of grouping is exactly what he did with his “lesbian mother” and “gay father” groups.

It should be noted that the analyses also fail to distinguish family structure from family stability for single mothers; this group included both continuously single mothers and those single mothers who had previously experienced a divorce.

The paper employs an unusual method to measure the sexual orientation of the respondents’ parents. Even if the analyses had distinguished family stability from family structure, this paper and its accompanying study could not actually directly examine the impact of having a gay or lesbian parent on child outcomes because the interpretation of the measurement of parental sexual orientation is unclear. The author acknowledges as much when he states:

It is, however, very possible that the same-sex romantic relationships about which the respondents report were not framed by those respondents as indicating their own (or their parent’s own) understanding of their parent as gay or lesbian or bisexual in sexual orientation. Indeed, this is more a study of the children of parents who have had (and in some cases, are still in) same-sex relationships than it is one of children whose parents have self-identified or are ‘‘out’’ as gay or lesbian or bisexual.

Respondents were asked whether their parents had ever had a same-sex relationship. The author then identifies mothers and fathers as “lesbian” or “gay” without any substantiation of parental sexual orientation either by respondents or their parents. Given the author’s stated caveats, it is both inappropriate and factually incorrect for him to refer to these parents as “gay” or “lesbian” throughout the paper.

We are very concerned about the academic integrity of the peer review process for this paper as well as its intellectual merit. We question the decision of Social Science Research to publish the paper, and particularly, to publish it without an extensive, rigorous peer review process and commentary from scholars with explicit expertise on LGBT family research. The methodologies used in this paper and the interpretation of the findings are inappropriate. The publication of this paper and the accompanying commentary calls the editorial process at Social Science Research, a well-regarded, highly cited social science journal (ranking in the top 15% of Sociology journals by ISI), into serious question. We urge you to publicly disclose the reasons for both the expedited peer review process of this clearly controversial paper and the choice of commentators invited to submit critiques. We further request that you invite scholars with specific expertise in LGBT parenting issues to submit a detailed critique of the paper and accompanying commentaries for publication in the next issue of the journal.

Bolding mine. See link for the list of 200 professionals who signed the letter.
Reply

#10
It's a travesty of academic fraud, and unfortunately we'll probably have to deal with the hate-mongers citing it for decades.
Reply



Forum Jump:


Recently Browsing
1 Guest(s)

© 2002-2024 GaySpeak.com